Knowledge sharing in heterogeneous collaborations – a longitudinal investigation of a cross-cultural research collaboration in nanoscience

Research Paper

Steffen Kanzler

Abstract

In times of globalization and rapidly developing R&D systems, the importance of international collaborative research activities increases, leading to a growing number of heterogeneous collaborations. Especially considering the cultural diversity of such collaborations, intensive cross-cultural knowledge sharing becomes a prerequisite for collaborative success. This paper investigates personal and cultural incentives and barriers influencing the intention to share knowledge of Chinese and German collaborators in an academic setting, employing a linear regression analysis and Chow tests. We can demonstrate that the factors sense of self-worth, loss of knowledge power, guanxi, and face-saving have an influence on an individual’s intention to share knowledge. Further, we find significant differences in our Chinese and German subgroups that can be related to cultural impacts. The obtained results provide practical and theoretical implications for the improvement of cross-cultural knowledge sharing in collaborative R&D project.

 

 

1 Introduction

Nowadays, researchers are facing highly complex problems, rapidly changing technologies and a dynamic growth of knowledge, due to the expansion of science on several axes, e.g. geographical, economical, multidisciplinary and multinational axis (Galison and Hevly, 1992). Often, individual academic scientists can no longer provide all of the expertise and resources necessary to address large research projects (Hara et al., 2003). Furthermore, these characteristics of modern research encourage scientists to get involved in collaborative research (Sooho and Bozeman, 2005). Generally, research collaborations1 can emerge between companies (C-C), companies and universities/research institutes (C-U) or universities (U-U).2

In particular, increasing global competition disposes companies to take advantage of synergy effects by intensifying global collaboration (Lam, 1997). Thereby, growth, expansion, exchange and generation of knowledge and technology represent the main reasons for joint C-C research activities (Campione, 2003; Odenthal et al., 2002).

The intentions of collaborators to strive for C-U collaboration differ. In addition to capitalizing on cost savings and access to the latest technology, companies utilize this kind of collaboration to open up cost-effective recruiting channels, to access laboratory usage, to share risks for basic research and to stabilize long term research projects (Azarloff, 1982; Bonaccorsi and Piccaluga, 1994; Cyert and Goodman, 1997; Rohrbeck and Arnold, 2006). In contrast, universities engaging in C-U collaborations strive for the enhancement of teaching, followed by achievement of funding and enhancement of reputation. Further motivations supporting collaborative behavior are to be found in the possibility of exchanging knowledge with industrial researchers, access to empirical data and job opportunities for graduates (Hurmelinna, 2004; Meyer-Krahmer and Schmoch, 1998).

However, in all types of collaborations and especially in academic ones knowledge sharing represents a main incentive e.g. by means of getting access to external knowledge on the one hand. On the other hand knowledge sharing is a prerequisite for successful collaboration (Hara et al., 2003; Niedergassel and Leker, 2008; Qian et al., 2008). Knowledge sharing has been in the focus of research for more than a decade and can be defined as the deployment of knowledge from a source to a recipient in communication (Berends et al., 2006). Following Nonaka, we define knowledge as justified true belief (Nonaka, 1994). Since knowledge is subjective and related to an individual’s experiences, knowledge sharing is embedded in a certain cognitive and behavioral context (Michailova and Hutchings, 2006). Qian et al. identified personal and cultural factors that impact on knowledge sharing (Qian et al., 2008). Niedergassel developed a conceptual framework with influencing factors for knowledge sharing in collaborative R&D projects (Niedergassel, 2009). He hypothesizes an influence of knowledge tacitness, knowledge newness, physical proximity, frequency of personal communication, trust between partners, pre-existing relationships, interdependency of partners, redundancy of knowledge sets and closeness of partners on knowledge sharing (Niedergassel, 2009).

While knowledge sharing in C-C collaborations has been widely discussed in the existing body of literature (Abrams, 2003; Cantner and Meder, 2007; Hansen, 1999; Hansen, 2002; Kaser and Miles, 2002; Lam, 1997; Levin and Cross, 2004; Reagans and McEvily, 2003), less effort has been made in analyzing knowledge sharing in academic collaborations (Hara et al., 2003; Niedergassel, 2009). In times of globalization and rapidly developing R&D systems, academic researchers increasingly strive for geographically distributed collaborations (Galison and Hevly, 1992; Hara et al., 2003). This leads to a constantly growing number of heterogeneous collaboration.3 Generally, heterogeneous collaborations are characterized by an inequality of the collaborating partners. Heterogeneity can occur on several dimensions. First, depending on contract situations between collaborators, unequally distributed hierarchy can cause heterogeneity. Second, heterogeneity can arise in research disciplines, e.g. when researchers from different scientific backgrounds are working on interdisciplinary projects. Third, the geographic distribution of the partners’ research basis can cause heterogeneity. Fourth, company and/or national culture can differ between collaborating partners, leading to heterogeneity.

In sight of the discussed increase in geographically distributed collaborative partnerships, especially cultural heterogeneity can cause serious difficulties in collaborative knowledge sharing activities. Thus, the understanding of cultural influences on knowledge-sharing behavior is gaining importance. Still, only a few studies analyzed cross-cultural knowledge sharing and they exclusively focused on C-C collaborations. Chow et al., for instance, analyzed the impact of collectivism versus individualism on the knowledge sharing behavior of Chinese and the U.S. American individuals (Chow et al., 2000). Similarly, Michailova and Hutchings compared knowledge sharing in Russia and China focusing on collectivism/individualism and universalism/particularism (Michailova and Hutchings,2006). Zhang et al. on the other hand investigated the impact of in-group/out-group affiliation on knowledge sharing in a cross-cultural setting (Zhang et al., 2008), which Chow indicated as well (Chow et al., 2000). Referring to Chow, Michailova, and Hutchings, Ardichvili emphasized the importance of the cultural factors collectivism/individualism, ingroup/out-group orientation, fear of losing face, and the importance of status and power distance, in his research on culture-specific barriers to knowledge sharing in China, Russia and Brazil (Ardichvili et al., 2006).

Contributing by filling the research gap regarding cross-cultural knowledge sharing in academic settings, we investigate different intentions to share knowledge in the first Chinese-German research collaboration on Nanoscience. Particularly, we focus on personal and cultural factors impacting the collaborators’ intention to share knowledge, employing a longitudinal research approach.

In the course of this paper, we first describe our research framework. Second, we present cultural factors differing between Chinese and German societies. Third, the Social Exchange Theory will be discussed and used to explain knowledge sharing behavior. Afterwards, we will present our hypotheses concerning the factors potentially impacting the intention to share knowledge. Subsequently, we will characterize our methodology, followed by a presentation of the results. Finally, a discussion and conclusion will summarize the findings of this paper, providing recommendations and practical guidelines for improving the process of knowledge sharing.

 

 

2 The Transregional Collaborative Research Centre: a heterogeneous collaboration

The “Transregional Collaborative Research Centre” (TRR 61) represents the first academic Chinese-German research collaboration on Nanoscience and is entitled “Multilevel Molecular Assemblies: Structure, Dynamics and Function”. Participants within the TRR 61 are the University of Münster (Germany), the Centre for Nanotechnology (CeNTech), the Centre for Nonlinear Science (CeNoS), the Tsinghua University (Beijing, China), the Chinese Academy of Science, the Interdisciplinary Research Centre for Cooperative Functional Systems (FOKUS) and the Chinese National Centre for NanoScience & Technology (NCNST, Beijing/China). Inspired by natural systems and their properties, chemists, physicists, and biologists are working on the interdisciplinary field of functional molecular and nano object assemblies. Participants of the TRR 61 are hierarchically equal and their knowledge is accessible for everybody within the TRR 61. The TRR 61 demonstrates heterogeneity on the disciplinary, the cultural and the geographical dimension, representing an ideal opportunity to investigate cultural impacts on knowledge sharing activities.

 

 

3 Cultural differences between China and Germany

Based on an analysis of Geert Hofstede (5Dmodel) concerning five cultural dimensions (Power Distance Index, PDI; Individualism, IDV; Masculinity, MAS; Uncertainty Avoidance Index, UAI; Long-Term Orientation, LTO), 4 Germany and China feature opposed parameter values in all dimensions, except Masculinity. The scores of China and Germany in Hofstede´s 5D-model are presented in Figure 1.

The PDI of China (80) is higher than the PDI in Germany (35), indicating a higher level of inequality of power and wealth in the Chinese than in the German society. Moreover, in China subordinates are unlikely to approach and contradict their supervisor in a direct way,  while German subordinates will do so morelikely (Hofstede and Hofstede, 2007).

The IDV scores are considerably higher in Germany (67) than in China (20), meaning that the German society is oriented towards individualism and the Chinese society is oriented towards collectivism. Thus, in the individualistic Germany tasks always prevail over personal relations and vice versa in the collectivistic China. According to Hofstede, Chinese German cooperation shows differences in social and group orientation, respectively. Whereas the German managers’ way of thinking and operating is affected by individualism, Chinese managers orient their behavior towards collectivism (cf. IDV) (Valentine and Godkin, 2001). Basically, this effect is derived from utterly heterogeneous political orientations, as well as from the importance of the family, traditionally founded in the long history of China (Ho, 1976). On this basis it is conjecturable that the collaboration propensity will be more pronounced for Chinese (Birnholtz, 2007).

China and Germany show equal scores (66) in masculinity, representing equal occurrence of clearly distinct emotional gender roles. In contrast, considerable differences emerge in the factor UAI. The scores in the UAI are higher in Germany (65) than in China (30). This indicates that there are more formal laws, informal rules and work regulations controlling the rights and duties of individuals in Germany than in China. In countries showing a low degree of uncertainty avoidance like China, one believes that many problems can be resolved without rules and that rules should only be established if absolutely necessary. Furthermore, in countries with a high degree of uncertainty avoidance orientation individuals like to be always busy and hard working or at least like to be seen so, while in low uncertainty avoidance countries individuals are able to work hard when needed, but they are not “driven by an inner urge toward constant activity” (Hofstede and Hofstede, 2007).

The greatest difference between the Chinese and the German culture according to Hofstede is found in LTO. China has the highest score (118) of all countries and Germany (31) is ranked with a low LTO score. Hence, culturally based differences between China and Germany in the concept of time are expected. Thereby, in contrast to Germans, Chinese do not think about time in terms of “time is money”. Since in China time appears as a relatively unlimited and cheap good, Chinese are more focused on the long-term outcome rather than on obtaining short-term success as it is to be found in Western countries (Wilpert and Scharpf, 1990).

Besides the Hofstede dimensions, cross-cultural researchers emphasize further important factors for the Chinese culture, namely guanxi (simply translated as ‘personal connections/relationships’) and the concept of face (Easterby-Smith and Malina, 1999; Ho, 1976; Jarman, 2001; Jiwen and James, 2007; Qian et al., 2008; Wilpert and Scharpf, 1990). Further factors affecting cross-cultural collaborative effectiveness are differing concepts of quality, differing respect for age and hierarchy and the use of third language communication (mainly English) (Wilpert and Scharpf, 1990). According to previous research on crosscultural knowledge sharing, e.g. studies conducted by Ardichvili, Michailova and Hutchings or Zhang, we argue that guanxi and the concept of face exert main impacts on knowledge sharing processes (Ardichvili et al., 2006; Michailova and Hutchings, 2006; Zhang et al., 2008). Thus, guanxi and the concept of face are discussed in detail in the following paragraphs.

Guanxi is mostly described as a form of interpersonal relationships and connections
unique to the Chinese culture. Due to the high value of harmony in the Confucian oriented
Chinese society, Chinese tend to emphasize good relationships in their social environment (Qian et al., 2008). Luo describes six traits that offer a comprehensive understanding of guanxi (Luo, 1997). First, guanxi is based on a utilitarian concept and therefore bonds individuals by exchanging favors rather than feelings. A guanxi relation not necessarily involves friends, however, if possible friends are preferred (Dunning and Kim, 2007). Ties based on guanxi are easily broken when they are not perceived to help in achieving goals. Second, guanxi means reciprocal exchange of favors and frequently tends to favor the weaker relation partner (Alston, 1989). Third, guanxi is transferable in the way that if A has guanxi with B, and A has guanxi with C, he can suggest B to C or vice versa. Fourth, guanxi is operating on the individual level and thus a highly personal concept. Hereby, trust, honesty, reciprocity, respect and social status are the essential features (Davies et al.,1995). In China, interpersonal loyalty is often more important than organizational affiliation or legal status (Dunning and Kim, 2007). Fifth, guanxi is directed to long-term interpersonal associations and interactions. Sixth and lastly, guanxi has an intangible quality, i.e. individuals who share guanxi ties are committed to each other by an “informal and unwritten code of trust, forbearance, reciprocity and equity” (Dunning and Kim, 2007). Disrespecting the virtues of guanxi can easily cause serious damage to an individual’s social standing and respectability.

The social standing of an individual is closely connected to the amount of ‘face’ an individual can claim for him/herself. Even though the concept of face is universally applicable to rank an individual’s standing in his social environment, the Chinese interpretation of face is specifically oriented to status and fixed role behavior (Wilpert and Scharpf, 1990). According to Leung and Chan, face is the “respect, pride and dignity of an individual as a consequence of his/her social achievements and the practice of it” (Leung and Chan, 2003). Cardon and Scott argue that face in China is an essential component of communication and relates to a person’s image and status within a social structure, while Westerners’ view of face is fairly simple and separated from communication (Cardon and Scott, 2003). Face has versatile characteristics. First, an individual’s face has a certain quantity, which can be increased by hard work, benefiting society, superior intellectual knowledge, accumulation of wealth and exemplary behavior, for instance (Brunner et al., 1989). Second, face has a positional aspect, i.e. the face position of individuals is generated by their social network and connections (Hwang, 1982). The larger the network and the more powerful the members connected to an individual the higher the face position. Third, face has a moral dimension, representing the confidence of society in the integrity of an individual’s character (Leung and Chan, 2003). Fourth, face has a dimension related to one’s prestige and reputation achieved through success and ostentation (Brunner et al., 1989). Fifth, in social interactions Chinese generally focus on saving face, giving face and avoiding a loss of face to others always under the unwritten law of reciprocity (Leung and Chan, 2003; Qian et al., 2008; Wilpert and Scharpf, 1990). Sixth and lastly, face can be transferred, i.e. buying face or borrowing face is a common praxis meaning that an individual may ask another one with a high social standing to intervene on his behalf, where the individual has not enough face (Cardon and Scott, 2003). Concluding, one has to note that Chinese collaboration partners might use face-related communication strategies to save or give face to others, e.g. indirectness, intermediaries on the one hand and praising or requests on the other (Cardon and Scott, 2003). Despite the critics to Hofstede’s survey and dimensions (for instance: Baskerville, R.F. (2003)) his framework has found broad acceptance and is often applied in academic research.

 

 

4 Research construct and hypothesis

The use of collaboration as a tool of science became an essential prerequisite particularly in “big science”, which is characterized by large-scale projects dealing with complex, rapidly changing problems and dynamic and highly specialized knowledge (Galison and Hevly, 1992; Hara et al., 2003). Moving from closed research to open research or even open innovation approaches, external knowledge sourcing and knowledge sharing become important requirements for universities (Lichtenthaler and Ernst, 2006). Often such collaborations are affected by “diversity of nationality, gender, ethnicity or profession” (Melin, 2000). Especially geographically distributed collaborators have to cope with further specific challenges, such as providing effective communication channels (e-mail, computer-networks, phone calls, etc.) and overcoming difficulties in project coordination to assure success (Birnholtz, 2007; Cummings and Kiesler, 2003; Cummings and Kiesler, 2005; Finholt, 2003). Otherwise, ideas or information pertaining to research and measuring instruments cannot be exchanged. However, sharing knowledge across long distances still remains complicated (McFadyen and Cannella Jr, 2005).

 

Social Exchange Theory and knowledge sharing

Whenever, deciding whether to participate in knowledge sharing activities, rational individuals will consider costs and benefits of such interactions (Qian et al., 2008). Therefore, the Social Exchange Theory (SET) can be employed to explain such situations. The SET argues that the exchange between individuals is a fundamental form of behavior and based on cost-benefit principles (Homans, 1961). Furthermore, Homans introduced psychological concepts like expectations and rewards and Blau introduced the concept of social reward, bridging the gap between individuals and society (Blau, 1964). Thibaut and Kelley propose that e.g. anticipated reciprocity and expected gain in reputation motivate individuals to participate in social exchange activities (Thibaut and Kelley, 1959). In contrast to economic exchange, social exchange occurs without specific obligations, i.e. roles or contracts. Thus, individuals do others a favor via such exchanges with the expectation of some future return, even without having a definite guarantee of this return. These characteristics match the knowledge-sharing concept. Hence, we argue that knowledge sharing could be regarded as a kind of social exchange.

The SET is often applied to knowledge sharing processes as a theoretical basis (Bock et
al., 2005; Niedergassel, 2009). Kankanhalli employed SET to investigate individual behavior in knowledge sharing (Kankanhalli et al., 2005). She focused on ‘costs and benefits’ according to SET for the analysis of incentives and barriers in knowledge sharing. Chua for instance employs a multi-person game-theoretic framework, however, he emphasizes reciprocity in knowledge sharing, declaring consistency with SET (Chua, 2003). Constant et al., using SET, argue that self-interest is an impeding factor for knowledge sharing (Constant et al., 1994). Bartol and Srivastava analyze how to design effective rewards for knowledge sharing via social exchange (Bartol and Srivastava, 2002).

Employing SET in our investigation of knowledge sharing behavior we conduct an economic analysis of noneconomic social exchanges (Emerson, 1976), thus we argue that applying the terms incentives and barriers for knowledge sharing as a noneconomic social exchange instead of the economic exchange terms benefits and costs is more appropriate. Hereby, we especially focus on individuals’ personal incentives and barriers, and cultural impacts that could enhance or reduce their intention to share knowledge. Particularly, we developed four hypotheses.

In literature, the sense of self-worth seems to be the main incentive for an individual to share knowledge. Individuals are more willing to participate in knowledge-sharing activities if they believe that their contribution is valued by others (Cabrera and Cabrera, 2002). Since participants can evaluate the usefulness of their knowledge through feedback in knowledge sharing activities, they can achieve an enhancement of their feeling of selfworth (Bock et al., 2005; Qian et al., 2008). Due to the individualistic orientation of the German culture, we argue that the sense of selfworth is more important to Germans than to Chinese.

Besides, giving and receiving feedback as a facilitator of knowledge sharing should be more direct and distinctive in Germany due to the lower power distance index. Thus we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 1: The sense of self-worth has a stronger positive influence on the intention to share knowledge in the German group than in the Chinese group.

On the contrary, a main barrier could be the loss of knowledge power caused by sharing of an individual’s unique knowledge. Previous studies suggested that individuals might be afraid to lose their competitive advantage by sharing knowledge, which they gained little by little throughout experience, failure and frustration and which enables them to exceed their colleagues’ performance (Kankanhalli et al., 2005; Qian et al., 2008). Although knowledge sharing could benefit themselves and the project, those might hold onto their knowledge if they believe to receive greater benefits by doing so (Davenport and Prusak, 2000). Due to the German tendency towards individualism, where everybody looks after himself and individual success is often more important than group success we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 2: The loss of knowledge power has a stronger negative influence on the intention to share knowledge in the German group than in the Chinese group.

In addition, cultural differences can cause difficulties and asymmetry in knowledge sharing (Lam, 1997; Zhang et al., 2008). Due to the different ways in which knowledge and skills are generated, organized, shared and utilized in different societal settings, one can expect an impact of culture when it comes to cross-cultural knowledge sharing (Lam, 1997; Zhang et al., 2008). Interpersonal networks and connections have an important influence on knowledge sharing (Weir and Hutchings, 2005). Further, social ties, including trust and rapport have a positive impact on knowledge sharing (Qian et al., 2008). Besides, Kotlarsky and Oshri argued that guanxi would promote knowledge sharing between partners (Kotlarsky and Oshri, 2005). A study conducted by Qian et al. in China demonstrates that guanxi orientation has a positive relationship with the intention to share knowledge (Qian et al., 2008). Since Chinese are very eager to maintain good relationships with people in their environment, they have a high guanxi orientation. Thus we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 3: The guanxi orientation has a stronger positive influence on the intention to share knowledge in the Chinese group than in the German group.

The amount of face an individual has can vary constantly (Ho, 1976). During the course of social interactions like knowledge sharing an individual’s face could be enhanced or diminished (Qian et al., 2008). Ardichvili et al. proposed that the desire of face saving is a barrier in knowledge sharing processes (Ardichvili et al., 2006). Accordingly, Qian et al. found a negative influence of face saving on the intention to share knowledge in their study (Qian et al., 2008). Individuals could be afraid that the knowledge they intend to share might be incorrect. Hence, sharing incorrect knowledge displays their ignorance and would thereby cause a loss of face. Therefore, individuals who try to save face would probably not participate in knowledge sharing activities. Furthermore, in order to save face people might restrict their behavior even to the extent of avoiding contact with others (Qian et al., 2008). Since the concept and the consequences of face are a more salient characteristic of the Chinese culture, we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 4: Face saving has a stronger negative influence on the intention to share knowledge in the Chinese group than in the German group.

Figure 2 summarizes the discussed hypotheses.

 

 

5 Data collection and measures

Due to the fact that our research project is part of the research objective TRR 61 itself, we have a unique opportunity to gather data on a chronological sequence of activities that occur throughout the collaboration. A standardized online questionnaire was developed in a two stage process. First, a literature review was performed to identify adequate constructs. In the questionnaire, we used existing scale items from previous studies where applicable and adapted these to the context of cross-cultural academic collaboration where necessary. Regarding the factor sense of self-worth, we employed the scale of Bock et al. (Bock et al., 2005). Loss of knowledge power was measured by the scale of Kankanhalli (Kankanhalli et al., 2005). Guanxi orientation was measured by the scale of Zuo (Zuo, 2002). We reformulated two of the six items to adapt them to the cross-cultural context. Concerning the factor face saving we used the scale introduced by Cheung et al. (Cheung et al., 2001). Finally, we employed the three-item scale by Ryu to measure the intention to share knowledge (Ryu et al., 2003). The response format was a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 ´I strongly disagree´ to 5 ´I strongly agree´).

Second, a pretest was performed by sending the questionnaire to selected university scientists, resulting in minimal changes (see Appendix for an overview of used items and constructs; the original questionnaire contained additional items not presented in this paper). All TRR 61 scientists were approached by personalized emails. A reminding email was sent out after 20 days, a second reminder was sent out after another 20 days and the survey was terminated 60 days after our first approach. Overall, we could obtain 49 responses, representing a response rate of 80%. 6 datasets had to be eliminated due to incomplete answers, leading to a final sample size of N = 43 (nChina = 17; nGermany = 26).

 

 

6 Analysis and results

In the first step of our analysis, we conducted a factor analysis to determine the structure of the employed constructs. Unidimensionality of the constructs was assessed employing an exploratory factor analysis. Cronbach’s alpha values were used to evaluate the reliability of the measures (Cronbach, 1951). We could show unidimensionality for all constructs except face saving, which did not exceed the commonly suggested threshold value of .70 (see Appendix for factor loadings and Cronbach’s alphas). However, to maintain the richness of the analysis, we decided not to further purify these constructs. Besides, we assessed the discriminant validity of the constructs by comparing variance extracted (VE) percentage with the squares of the correlation estimates, as proposed by Fornell and Larcker (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). Discriminant validity could only be demonstrated for loss of knowledge power and guanxi orientation. However, the global criteria and more than 50% of partial criteria are met; thus, all constructs are retained for further analysis.5 The goodness-of-fit can be considered acceptable for the overall model (GFI = .969; AGFI = .960; RMR = .072).

Harman’s single factor test was employed to address the issue of common method bias. The test indicates substantial common method bias if only one single factor emerges from exploratory factor analysis or one general factor accounts for more than 50% of the covariance between the measures. We could find neither of these conditions applying Harman’s single factor test to our sample.

In the second step, we constructed linear regression models with the intention to share knowledge as the dependent variable for each factor, i.e. sense of self-worth, loss of knowledge power, face saving and guanxi orientation. Our hypotheses indicate differing impacts of the factors on the intention to share knowledge depending on the cultural background. Accordingly, a statistical method to test the effect of a variable on the direction or the strength of a relation between an independent and a dependent variable is a moderator analysis (Baron and Kenny, 1986). In our moderator analysis the dependent variable is the intention to share knowledge, the independent variables are sense of self-worth, loss of knowledge power, face saving and guanxi orientation and the moderating variable is nationality. Further, we followed the framework for identifying moderator variables developed by Sharma (Sharma et al., 1981). According to Hambrick, we conducted Chow tests to test our hypotheses (Hambrick and Lei,1985).

The Chow test for homogeneity of regression results is a straightforward method to observe differences in regression results and found broad acceptance (Hambrick and Lei, 1985). First, we ran separate regressions for the two subsamples. Second, we ran the regressions for the total sample. The values of interest were the sum of squared errors for the total sample and the subsamples. If the errors obtained from the subsamples are small relative to the errors of the total sample, a moderating effect can be assumed. In Table 1, results for the four different regression models are reported.

For the interpretation of the Chow test we used a F-statistic table (Backhaus, 2006). If significant differences are found, nationality can be considered a moderator that operates through the error term, often also called ‘homologizer’ (Sharma et al., 1981). All variance inflation factors (VIFs) in our linear regression models were well below the widely accepted threshold value of 10 (Hair, 2006).

Regarding the sense of self-worth, splitting into subsamples results in an improvement of the adj. R2 value in the German subsample and a decrease of the R2 value in the Chinese sample. The standardized regression coefficient is higher in regression model of the German subsample. However, the Chow test was not significant, thus Hypothesis 1 has to be rejected. The sense of self-worth equally influences the intention to share knowledge in the two subsamples.

For the factor loss of knowledge power we find considerable differences. Again, we find an improved adj. R2 value in the German and a decreased adj. R2 value in the Chinese subsample. Further, we can find the hypothesized negative influence of loss of knowledge power on the intention to share knowledge in the total sample and the German subsample, but not in the Chinese subsample, where this regression model is not significant. The Chow test is significant at p < .01, supporting Hypothesis 2. As expected, the loss of knowledge power has a negative influence on the intention to share knowledge in the German subsample, while the loss of knowledge power has no influence on the intention to share knowledge in the Chinese subsample.

Separation into subsamples regarding guanxi orientation results in an improvement of the model fit in both subsamples. The adjusted adj. R2 rises from .705 to .757 in the Chine se subsample and to .711 in the German sample, respectively. The standardized regression coefficients as well increase from .844 to .879 and .850 in the Chinese and German subsamples. The Chow test is significant at the level of p < .05, providing support for Hypothesis 3. Thus we demonstrated that the influence of guanxi orientation has a stronger influence on the intention to share knowledge in the Chinese than in the German group.

Lastly, we find an improvement of the adj. R2 value in the Chinese subsample and a decrease of adj. R2 in the German subsample, segmenting into subgroups in the regression model with face saving as the independent variable. Furthermore, face saving has only a significant influence on the intention to share knowledge in the Chinese subsample. Nevertheless, none of the three regression models demonstrates the hypothesized negative influence. Additionally the Chow test is not significant, disproving Hypothesis 4.

 

 

7 Discussion and conclusion

This study offers several interesting findings regarding personal and cultural impacts on the process of knowledge sharing in crosscultural collaborative R&D projects. Particularly, the regression models allow us to identify success factors and barriers influencing the intention to share knowledge of collaborating researchers, contributing to the existing body of literature by considering an academic and cross-cultural perspective. Furthermore, we find considerable differences in the influencing factors between Chinese and German groups that can be related to cultural impacts.

The sense of self-worth demonstrates an equally positive influence in both subsamples on an individual’s intention to share knowledge. However, while the Germans’ individualistic orientation and the low power distance index as a facilitator of giving feedback could enhance the importance of self-worth in Germany, the Chinese desire to gain face as a facilitator to increase one’s sense of self worth could explain the importance of this construct in the Chinese group. Nevertheless, we could show the importance of sense of self-worth for the intention to share knowledge in a cross-cultural academic setting, supporting the findings of Bock et al. and Qian et al. (Qian et al., 2008). Hence, researchers in collaborative projects should establish frequent feedback rounds, in which past knowledge-sharing activities are analyzed in a way that individuals see how their contribution in knowledge-sharing processes has improved the projects’ performance. Such discussions would allow participants to increase their sense of self-worth and would further have a positive impact on their intention to share knowledge, enhancing future knowledge-sharing activities.

Significant differences emerge in the regression model with loss of knowledge power as the independent variable. In the German subsample we could demonstrate a negative influence of loss of knowledge power on the intention to share knowledge. The German society is characterized by an individualistic orientation. We argue that this orientation enhances the fear of losing competitive advantages, even in academic settings. On the contrary, in the Chinese subsample loss of knowledge power has no significant influence on the intention to share knowledge. However, previous research in a Chinese setting could show that loss of knowledge power has a negative influence in knowledge sharing processes in an economic setting (Bock et al., 2005; Qian et al., 2008). Interestingly, we cannot support Qian’s findings in our academic setting, implying that Chinese academic researchers are not afraid to lose competitive advantages through knowledge sharing. Instead, by sharing superior intellectual knowledge scientists could gain face, which is highly important in Chinese societies (Brunner et al.,1989). Besides, Kankanhalli et al. could not prove their hypothesized negative impact of loss of knowledge power in knowledge sharing processes in their setting either (Kankanhalli et al., 2005). Further examinations regarding setting impacts could give new impetus to the concept’s continuous development.

As hypothesized, the guanxi orientation has a significantly stronger positive influence on the intention to share knowledge in the Chinese subgroup. Furthermore, in the Chinese group guanxi orientation has the strongest influence on the intention to share knowledge in all regression models, highlighting the outstanding social relationship orientation. Thus, we could demonstrate that a cultural factor has a larger impact on knowledge sharing processes than personal factors, supporting findings of Qian et al.. As Qian et al. further pointed out, Chinese try to create a harmonious atmosphere, which enables knowledge sharing in the first place and facilitates the building of reciprocal relationships (Qian et al., 2008). Surprisingly, the standardized correlation coefficient of .850 in the German subsample is also considerably high. Accordingly, social relations have an important influence on the intention to share knowledge in the German subsample, too. Niedergassel, for instance, demonstrated in a German academic setting that knowledge sharing is enhanced if the relationship between collaborators is particularly close (Niedergassel and Leker, 2009). However, the Chinese guanxi orientation is a unique phenomenon and has to be closely considered when striving for collaboration with Chinese partners. Maintaining a good relationship to Chinese partners by exchanging favors and following the unwritten law of reciprocity is a key strategy for successful collaboration (Davies et al., 1995; Dunning and Kim, 2007; Lockett, 1988; Valentine and Godkin, 2001; Zhang et al., 2008).

Finally, we could not demonstrate the hypothesized negative effect of face saving on the
intention to share knowledge in neither of the subgroups. Furthermore, the Chow test is not significant, disproving our hypothesis. Thus, we cannot support the findings of Qian et al., who could demonstrate a negative influence of face-saving and a positive influence of face gaining on the intention to share (Qian et al., 2008). Therefore, we argue that multiple facets of the concept of the face have to be considered. However, Zhang et al. pointed out that saving face is less important to Chinese when interacting with foreigners since one can only lose face to members of one’s social environment (Zhang et al., 2008). Accordingly, Ardichvili et al. argue that the impact of the concept of the face was weaker than expected in their study, too (Ardichvili et al., 2006). They suggest, that the Chinese feel rather comfortable asking questions and contributing to discussions if such interactions improve project performance (Ardichvili et al., 2006). Further, Ardichvili et al. reason that face-saving is more a concern for older Chinese (Ardichvili et al., 2006). Nevertheless, we still believe that the concept of face has a strong impact on any interaction in collaborative activities with Chinese partners. Thus, we emphasize that one should carefully focus on the consequences and implications of the face when collaborating with Chinese partners. For instance, giving face, i.e. doing something that enhances someone else’s reputation or prestige by praising, gift-giving or concessions can improve the performance of collaborations with Chinese partners (Cardon and Scott, 2003).

While offering many interesting findings, this study also possesses some limitations requiring consideration. Our study is based on a comparatively small sample size and we focused on a knowledge generation oriented academic setting, thus generalizing our results to economic situations may not be appropriate. However, we will conduct qualitative interviews to support the findings of our quantitative  analysis. Besides, we especially focused on Chinese cultural factors, though future research should investigate cultural characteristics of western societies that might influence knowledge sharing processes. Generally, strategies, non-monetary rewarding and incentive systems facilitating knowledge sharing should be developed and discussed more deeply.

Despite the limitations we still believe to make a valuable contribution to the existing body of literature on cross-cultural knowledge sharing in innovation, technology and collaboration management, particularly considering the academic partners´ point of view and the increasing importance of collaborative activities with partners from China.

 

 

Acknowledgment

The author would like to thank the German Research Foundation (DFG) for financial support of this project within the Transregional Collaborative Research Centre TRR 61.

 

 

References

Abrams, L.C., Cross, R., Lesser, E. and Levin, D. Z. (2003): Nurturing Interpersonal Trust in Knowledge-Sharing Networks, Academy of Management Executive, 14(4), p.64-77.

Alston, J.P. (1989): Wa, Guanxi, and Inhwa: Managerial Principles in Japan, China, and Korea, Business Horizons, 32(2), p. 26-31.

Ardichvili,A.,Maurer,M., Li,W.,Wentling,T.,and Stuedemann, R. (2006): Cultural Influences on Knowledge Sharing through Online Communities of Practice, Journal of Knowledge Management, 10, p. 94-107.

Azarloff, L.V. (1982): Industry-University Collaboration: How to Make It Work, Research Management, 25(3), p. 31-35.

Backhaus, K.,B.Erichson, W.Plinke and R.Weber (2006): Tabellenanhang, in: Multivariate Analysemethoden: Eine anwendungsorientierte Einführung, Springer, Berlin, p. 809-813.

Baron, R.M., and Kenny, D.A.(1986): The Moderator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations, Journal of personality and social psychology, 51(6), p. 1173-1182.

Bartol, K.M.and Srivastava, A.(2002): Encouraging Knowledge Sharing: The Role of Organizational Reward Systems, Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies, 9(1), p. 64-76.

Baskerville, R.F.(2003): Hofstede never studied culture, Accounting, Organizations and society, 28(1), p. 1-14.

Berends, H., van der Bij, H., Debackere, K., and Weggeman, M. (2006): Knowledge Sharing Mechanisms in Industrial Research, R&D Management, 37(1), p. 85-95.

Birnholtz, J.P. (2007): When Do Researchers Collaborate? Towards a Model of Collaboration Propensity in Science and Engineering Research, Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 58(14), p. 2226-2239.

Blau, P.M. (1964): Exchange and Power in Social Life, Wiley: New York.

Bock, G.-W., Zmud, R.W., Young-Gul, K., and Jae-Nam, L.(2005): Behavioral Intention Formatting in Knowledge Sharing: Examining the Roles of Extrinsic Motivators, Social-Psychological Forces and Organizational Climate, MIS Quarterly, 29(1), p. 87-111.

Bonaccorsi, A. and Piccaluga, A.(1994): A Theoretical Framework for the Evaluation of University-Industry Relationships, R&D Management, 24(3), p. 229-247.

Brunner, J.A., Chen, J., Sun, C., and Zhou, N. (1989): The Role of Guanxi in Negotiations in the Pacific Basin, Journal of Global Marketing, 3(2), p. 7-24.

Cabrera, Á.and Cabrera, E.F.(2002): Knowledge-SharingDilemmas, Organization Studies, 23(5), p. 687-710.

Campione, T.J.(2003): Making Research Collaborations Succeed, Research Technology Management, 46(4), p. 12-15.

Cantner, U. and Meder, A. (2007): Technological Proximity and the Choice of Cooperation Partner, Journal of Economic Interaction and Coordination, 2(1), p. 46-65.

Cardon, P.W. and Scott, J.C. (2003): Chinese Business Face: Communication Behaviors and Teaching Approaches, Business Communication Quarterly, 66(4), p. 9-22.

Cheung, F.M., Leung, K., Zhang, J.-X., Sun, H.-F., Gan, Y.-Q., Song, W.-Z., and Xie, D. (2001): Indigenous Chinese Personality Constructs: Is the Five-Factor Model Complete?, Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 32(4), p. 407-433.

Chow, C.W., Deng, F.J., and Ho, J.L. (2000): The Openness of Knowledge Sharing within Organizations: A Comparative Study of the United States and the People’s Republic of China, Journal of Management Accounting Research, 12(1), p. 65-95.

Chua, A. (2003): Knowledge Sharing: A Game People Play, Aslib Proceedings, 55(3), p. 117-129.

Constant, D., Sproull, L., and Kiesler, S. (1994): What’s Mine Is Ours, or Is It? A Study of Attitudes About Information Sharing, Information System Research, 5(4), p.400-421.

Cronbach, L.J. (1951): Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Tests, Psychometrika, 16(3), p. 297-334.

Cummings, J.N. and Kiesler, S.(2003): Multidisciplinary Scientific Collaborations, in: Proceedings of the International Conference on Information Systems: Seattle. p. 296-303.

Cummings,J.N.and Kiesler,S.(2005): Collaborative Research across Disciplinary and Organizational Boundaries, Social Studies of Science, 35(5), p. 703-722.

Cyert, R.M., and Goodman, P.S.(1997): Creating Effective University-Industry Alliances: An Organizational LearningPerspective, Organizational Dynamics, 25(4), p. 45-57.

Davenport, T.H. and Prusak, L. (2000): Working Knowledge: How Organizations ManageWhat They Know, Harvard Business School Press: Boston.

Davies, H., Leung, T.K.P., Luk, S.T.K., and Wong, Y.-h.(1995): The Benefits Of “Guanxi”: The Value of Relationships in Developing the Chinese Market, Industrial Marketing Management, 24(3), p. 207-214.

Dunning, J.H.and Kim, C.(2007): The Cultural Roots of Guanxi: An Exploratory Study, World Economy, 30(2), p. 329-341.

Easterby-Smith,M.and Malina,D.(1999): Cross-Cultural Collaborative Research: Toward Reflexivity, Academy of Management Journal, 42(1), p. 76-86.

Emerson, R. (1976): Social Exchange Theory, Annual Review of Sociology, 2, p. 335-362.

Finholt, T.A. (2003): Collaboratories as a New Form of Scientific Organization, Economics of Innovation & New Technology, 12(1), p. 5-26.

Fornell, C. and Larcker,D. (1981): Evaluating Structural Equation Models with Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error., Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1), p. 39-50.

Galison, P. and Hevly, B. (1992): The Many Faces of Big Science, in: Big Science: The Growth of Large Scale Research, Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA, p. 1-14.

Gronau, N. (2002): Kollaborative Engineering Communities – Architektur und Integrationsansätze, in: Proceedings der Multikonferenz Wirtschaftsinformatik, Mertens, P.: Nürnberg. p. 1-15.

Hair, J.F., Black, W.C., Babin, B.J., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L. (2006): Multivariate Data Analysis, Pearson Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ.

Hambrick, D.C., and Lei, D. (1985): Toward an Empirical Prioritization of Contingency Variables for Business Strategy, Academy of Management Journal, 28(4), p. 763-788.

Han, Y.(1997): Software Infrastructure for ConfigurableWorkflow Systems: A Model Driven Approach Based on Higher Order Object Nets and Corba, Wissenschaft undTechnik: Berlin.

Hansen, M.T. (1999): The Search-Transfer Problem: The Role of Weak Ties in Sharing Knowledge across Organization Subunits, Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(1), p. 82-111.

Hansen, M.T.(2002): Knowledge Networks: Explaining Effective Knowledge Sharing in Multiunit Companies, Organization Science, 13(3), p. 232-248.

Hara, N., Solomon, P., Kim, S.-L., and Sonnenwald, D.H.(2003): An EmergingView of Scientific Collaboration: Scientists’ Perspectives on Collaboration and Factors That Impact Collaboration, Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 54(10), p. 952-965.

Heinze, T. and Kuhlmann, S. (2006): Analysis of Heterogenous Collaboration in the German Research System with a Focus on Nanotechnology., Frauenhofer ISI discussion papers: Innovation system and policy analysis, (6), p. 1-22.

Ho, D.Y.-f. (1976): On the Concept of Face, The American Journal of Sociology, 81(4), p. 867-884.

Hofstede, G. and Hofstede, G.J.(2007): Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind, McGraw-Hill: New York.

Homans, G.C. (1961): Social Behaviour: Its Elementary Forms, in: International Library of Sociology and Social Reconstruction, Routledge and Kegan Paul, London.

Hurmelinna, P. (2004): Motivations and Barriers Related to University-Industry Collaboration – Approbability and the Principle of Publicity, UC Berkeley.

Hwang, C.H. (1982): Studies in Chinese Personality: A Critical Review, Bulletin of Educational Psychology,15, p. 227.

Jarman, F. (2001): Opportunities and Problems of Intercultural Communication and Cooperation for Small and Medium Enterprises,in:Synergien durch regionale Netzwerke / von lernenden Organisationen zur lernenden Region -Dokumentation des Projekts Netzwerk für Arbeit im Rahmen derGemeinschaftsinitiative Adapt der Europäischen Union, Wesely,W., Expressum Verlag, Hannover, p. 259-272.

Jiwen, S.L., and James, D.W.(2007): Guanxi as Impetus? Career Exploration in China and the United States, Career Development International, 12(1), p. 51-67.

Kankanhalli, A., Tan, B.C.Y., and Kwok-Kee, W. (2005): Contributing Knowledge to Electronic Knowledge Repositories: An Empirical Investigation, MIS Quarterly, 29(1), p. 113-143.

Kaser, P.A.W. and Miles, R.E.(2002): Understanding Knowledge Activists’ Successes and Failures, Long Range Planning, 35(1), p. 9-28.

Kotlarsky, J. and Oshri, I. (2005): Social Ties, Knowledge Sharing and Successful Collaboration in Globally Distributed System Development Projects, European Journal of Information Systems, 14(1), p. 37-48.

Lam, A.(1997): Embedded Firms, Embedded Knowledge: Problems of Collaboration and Knowledge Transfer in Global Cooperative Ventures, Organization Studies, 18(6), p. 973-997.

Leung, T.K.P. and Chan, R.Y.-K. (2003): Face, Favour and Positioning a Chinese PowerGame, European Journal of Marketing, 37(11/12), p. 1575-1598.

Levin, D.Z. and Cross, R. (2004): The Strength of Weak Ties You Can Trust: The Mediating Role of Trust in Effective Knowledge Transfer, Management Science, 50(11), p. 1477-1490.

Lichtenthaler, U. and Ernst, H.(2006): Attitudes to Externally Organising Knowledge Management Tasks: A Review, Reconsideration and Extension of the NIH Syndrome, R&D Management, 36(4), p. 367-386.

Lockett, M. (1988): Culture and the Problems of Chinese Management, Organization Studies, 9(4), p. 475-496.

Luo, Y. (1997): Guanxi and Performance of Foreign-Invested Enterprises in China: An Empirical Inquiry, Management International Review (MIR), 37(1), p. 51-70.

Mc Fadyen, M. A.and Cannella Jr, A. A.(2005): Knowledge Creation and the Location of University Research Scientists’ Interpersonal Exchange Relations: Within and Beyond the University, Strategic Organization, 3(2), p. 131-155.

Melin, G.(2000): Pragmatism and Self-Organization: Research Collaboration on the Individual Level, Research Policy, 29(1), p. 31-40.

Meyer-Krahmer, F. and Schmoch, U. (1998): Science-Based Technologies: University-Industry Interactions in Four Fields, Research Policy, 27(8), p. 835-851.

Michailova, S. and Hutchings, K. (2006): National Cultural Influences on Knowledge Sharing: A Comparison of China and Russia, Journal of Management Studies, 43(3), p. 383-405.

Müller, J.(2006): Kooperationen und die Wettbewerbsfähigkeit von Unternehmen, J.W. Goethe-Universität: Frankfurt am Main.

Niedergassel, B. and Leker, J. (2008): Tacit vs. Explicit – the Role of Different Knowledge Dimensions in Collaborative R&DProjects, in: Conference Proceedings XIX ISPIM: Tours.

Niedergassel, B. (2009): Knowledge Sharing in Collaborative R&D Projects, Institut für Betriebswirtschaftliches Management im Fachbereich Chemie und Pharmazie, WWU Münster.

Niedergassel, B.and Leker, J.(2009): Tacit and Explicit Knowledge Sharing in Collaborative R&DProjects, in: Conference Proceedings IAMOT: Orlando.

Nonaka, I. (1994): A Dynamic Theory of Organizational Knowledge Creation, Organization Science, 5(1),p.14-37.

Odenthal, S., Säubert, H., and Weishaar, A. (2002): Strategische Partnerschaften – Mehr Erfolg mit dem neuen Partnering-Ansatz, Gabler Verlag:Wiesbaden.

Qian, H., Davison, R.M., and Jibao, G. (2008): Impact of Personal and Cultural Factors on Knowledge Sharing in China, Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 25(3), p.451-471.

Reagans, R. and McEvily, B. (2003): Network Structure and Knowledge Transfer: The Effects of Cohesion and Range, Administrative Science Quarterly, 48(2), p. 240-267.

Rohrbeck, R. and Arnold, H.M. (2006): Making UniversityIndustry CollaborationWork – a Case Study on the Deutsche Telekom Laboratories Contrasted with Findings in Literature, in: Conference Proceedings XVII ISPIM Networks for Innovation: Athens, Greece. p. 1-11.

Ryu, S., Ho, S.H., and Han, I.(2003): Knowledge Sharing Behavior of Physicians in Hospitals, Expert Systems with Applications, 25, p. 113-122.

Sharma, S., Durand, R.M., and Gur-Arie, O. (1981): Identification and Analysis of ModeratorVariables, Journal of Marketing Research, 18(3), p. 291-300.

Lee, S. and Bozeman, B. (2005): The Impact of Research Collaboration on Scientific Productivity, Social Studies of Science, 35(5), p. 673-702.

Thibaut, J.W. and Kelley, H.H. (1959): The Social Psychology of Groups, Wiley: New York.

Valentine, S. and Godkin, L. (2001): United States Expatriates in China: The Impact of Cultural and Managerial Differences, International Journal of Management, 18(4), p. 482.

Weir, D. and Hutchings, K. (2005): Cultural Embeddedness and Contextual Constraints: Knowledge Sharing in Chinese and Arab Cultures, Knowledge and Process Management, 12(2), p. 89-98.

Wilpert, B. and Scharpf, S.Y. (1990): Intercultural Management- Joint Ventures in the People´s Republic of China, International Journal of Psychology, 25(5/6), p. 643.

Zhang, Q., Ribiere, V.M., and Thippaya, C. (2008): The Impact of Group Relation on Knowledge Sharing: A Cross-Cultural Study, in: Strategic Knowledge Management in Multinational Organizations, O´Sullivan, K., Information Science Reference, Hershey, p. 40-52.

Zuo, B. (2002): The Chinese Relation Orientation: Concept and Its Measurement, Journal of Central China Normal University (Humanities and Social Sciences), (1), p. 74-80

 

 

Appendix

Issues

Issue Select

Related Articles